People who know they are being watched behave differently than those not being watched (e.g., Aiello & Svec, 1993; Bond & Titus, 1983; Latané, 1981; Putz, 1975; Risko & Kingstone, 2011; Triplett, 1898; Zajonc, 1965).  For example, they are more generous (Kurzban, 2001; Satow, 1975) and more helpful (van Rompay, Vonk, & Fransen, 2009).  It has been hypothesized that this tendency is so deeply ingrained that even artificial cues of being watched impact behavior.  In a recent paper, I teamed up with Dr. William Pedersen, Dr. Adam Cohen, and Dr. Paul Andrews to systematically investigate the literature on surveillance cues using a technique called meta-analysis.  This allowed us to draw broad (if tentative) conclusions about the state of the literature.

The last decade has witnessed the introduction and development of a literature that seemingly supports this idea: Anonymous individuals shown mere images of watching eyes (or similarly artificial surveillance cues) behave more commendably, as if they are being watched by real people.  Exposing people to cues of being watched has ostensibly resulted in several desirable outcomes, such as increased generosity (e.g., Pfattheicher, 2015), reduced littering (e.g., Bateson et al., 2015), and increased voter turnout to an election (e.g., Panagopoulos, 2015). 

However, several studies have failed to replicate this surveillance cue effect.  Furthermore, significant results are often conditional on the methods of data analysis, participant or surveillance cue traits, or specific features of the environment.  A review of the literature reveals several qualifications, each only found in one or a few studies.  These include the findings that artificial surveillance cues affect the behavior of men, but not women (Rigdon, Ishii, Watabe, & Kitayama, 2009); that surveillance cues affect the way participants treat members of their group, but not the way they treat outsiders (Mifune, Hashimoto, & Yamagishi, 2010); that surveillance cues only affect participants who are high in chronic public self-awareness (Pfattheicher & Keller, 2015) or not angry (Horita & Takezawa, 2014); and that surveillance cues only work when they are shown briefly (Sparks & Barclay, 2013), or when the number of people in an area is low (Ekström, 2012) or, conversely, high (Bateson, Callow, Holmes, Redmond Roche, & Nettle, 2013).  While conditional effects are often illuminating, it is a cause for concern that the conditions on which surveillance cue effects seemingly depend differ from study to study.

The literature thus presents a mixed picture of artificial observation cues.  Several studies have failed to replicate surveillance cue effects, and many papers report mixed or qualified results.  The conditional surveillance cue effects reported by papers have rarely been replicated.  Therefore, one might wonder how strong the watching eyes effect is, and indeed whether or not it even exists.  My co-authors and I decided to conduct a meta-analysis, which is a combination of results from multiple studies of a given effect.  A meta-analysis can produce a more reliable and precise estimate of the true size of an effect than a single study can.

We decided to focus on generosity, the most frequently investigated outcome in surveillance cue studies.  Most of the generosity studies can be described as either charity donation or dictator game studies.  In a typical charity donation study, subjects are given money as payment for their participation and then asked if they would like to donate some or all of their money to a charity.  In a dictator game study, participants are assigned to groups of two.  One participant in each pair, the dictator, receives money and decides how to allocate it among him/herself and the second participant.  The second participant merely accepts what the dictator offers, if the dictator offers anything at all.

We were interested in whether participants who were shown surveillance cues were more generous than participants who were not shown surveillance cues.  Generosity was measured in two different ways: the amount of money that participants gave (‘amount given’), and the proportion of participants who gave something, rather than nothing (‘proportion who gave’).  We conducted two separate meta-analyses, one for both measurements of generosity.  The ‘amount given’ meta-analysis included data from 2,732 participants from 26 experiments, and the ‘proportion who gave’ meta-analysis included data from 19,512 participants from 27 experiments.

According to both meta-analyses, the generosity of participants who were shown surveillance cues was not significantly different from the generosity shown by participants who were not shown surveillance cues.  In other words, our meta-analyses found no evidence that artificial cues of being watched increase generosity, either by increasing how generous individuals are or by increasing the probability that individuals will show any generosity at all.  One important limitation of our study is that our meta-analyses did not take into account any of the qualifications described previously.  The importance of qualifications is therefore still an open question.  Confidence in conditional effects, as with any effect, should be apportioned to their ability to be replicated repeatedly with large samples.  Our meta-analyses aren’t the final word on surveillance cues and generosity, but they show that skepticism is warranted.


Stefanie Northover is a PhD student in social psychology at Arizona State University.  She is interested in the psychology of religion from evolutionary and cultural perspectives.

Original Article

Northover, S. B., Pedersen, W. C., Cohen, A. B., & Andrews, P. W. (in press). Artificial surveillance cues do not increase generosity: two meta-analyses. Evolution and Human Behavior.

References

Aiello, J. R., & Svec, C. M. (1993). Computer monitoring and work performance: Extending the social facilitation framework to electronic presence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 537-548. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1993.tb01102.x

Bateson, M., Callow, L., Holmes, J. R., Redmond Roche, M. L., & Nettle, D. (2013). Do images of ‘watching eyes’ induce behaviour that is more pro-social or more normative? A field experiment on littering. PLoS ONE, 8(12), e82055. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0082055

Bateson, M., Robinson, R., Abayomi-Cole, T., Greenlees, J., O’Connor, A., & Nettle, D. (2015). Watching eyes on potential litter can reduce littering: Evidence from two field experiments. PeerJ, 3:e1443. doi:10.7717/peerj.1443

Bond, C. F., Jr., & Titus, L. J. (1983). Social facilitation: A meta-analysis of 241 studies. Psychological Bulletin, 94(2), 265-292. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.94.2.265

Ekström, M. (2012). Do watching eyes affect charitable giving? Evidence from a field experiment. Experimental Economics, 15, 530-546. doi:10.1007/s10683-011-9312-6

Horita, Y., & Takezawa, M. (2014). Observation enhances third-party punishment only among people who were not hot-tempered. Letters on Evolutionary Behavioral Science, 5, 5-8. doi:10.5178/lebs.2014.28

Kurzban, R. (2001). The social psychophysics of cooperation: Nonverbal communication in a public goods game. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 25(4), 241-259. doi:10.1023/A:1012563421824

Latané, B. (1981). The psychology of social impact. The American Psychologist, 36, 343-356. doi:10.1037//0003-066X.36.4.343

Mifune, N., Hashimoto, H., & Yamagishi, T. (2010). Altruism toward in-group members as a reputation mechanism. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 109-117. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.09.004

Panagopoulos, C. (2015). A closer look at eyespot effects on voter turnout: Reply to Matland and Murray. Political Psychology. doi:10.1111/pops.12281

Pfattheicher, S. (2015). A regulatory focus perspective on reputational concerns: The impact of prevention-focused self-regulation. Motivation and Emotion, 39, 932-942. doi:10.1007/s11031-015-9501-2

Pfattheicher, S., & Keller, J. (2015). The watching eyes phenomenon: The role of a sense of being seen and public self-awareness. European Journal of Social Psychology, 45, 560–566. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2122

Putz, V. R. (1975). The effects of different modes of supervision on vigilance behaviour. British Journal of Psychology, 66(2), 157-160. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1975.tb01449.x

Rigdon, M., Ishii, K., Watabe, M., & Kitayama, S. (2009). Minimal social cues in the dictator game. Journal of Economic Psychology, 30, 358-367. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2009.02.002

Risko, E. F., & Kingstone, A. (2011). Eyes wide shut: Implied social presence, eye tracking and attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 291-296. doi:10.3758/s13414-010-0042-1

Satow, K. (1975). Social approval and helping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 11, 501-509. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(75)90001-3

Sparks, A., & Barclay, P. (2013). Eye images increase generosity, but not for long: The limited effect of a false cue. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 317-322. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.05.001  

Triplett, N. (1898). The Dynamogenic Factors in Pacemaking and Competition. The American Journal of Psychology, 9(4), 507-533. doi:10.2307/1412188

van Rompay, T. J. L., Vonk, D. J, & Fransen, M. L. (2009). The eye of the camera: Effects of security cameras on prosocial behavior. Environment and Behavior, 41(1), 60-74. doi:10.1177/0013916507309996

Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science, 149(3681), 269-274. doi:10.2307/1715944